Когницијата и теоријата за умот кај деца со нарушувања од аутистичен аспект

  • Lence Miloseva

Abstract

Нарушувањата од аутистичeн спектар се развојни нарушувања коисе огледаат во тешкотии во социјалните интеракции и комуникации, какои постоење на стереотипно однесување, интереси и активности. Со огледдека клиничката слика на нарушувањата од аутистичен спектар варира востепенот на сериозноста и тежината на симптомите, се смета за оправданода се зборува за спектар на развојни нарушувања. Во овој прегледен труд гиевалуиравме хипотезите за теоријата за умот во светло на истражувањатакои како предмет на проучување ги имале следните прашања: Дали децатасо аутистично нарушување развиваат концепти на теоријата за умот?Како можеме да објасниме зошто некои деца со аутистично нарушувањеуспешно ги решаваат задачите од теоријата за умот? Дали дефицитите вотеоријата за умот се сметаат за големи оштетувања кои ги карактеризираатаутистичните нарушувања? До денес не е предложена теорија која би гиобјаснила сите важни аспекти на аутистичните нарушувања или пак бибила единствена, унифицирана за сите аутистични нарушувања. Земајќипредвид дека во литературата се најзастапени теориите кои нарушувањатаод аутистичниот спектар ги толкуваат како дефицит на теоријата за умот,во трудот сe обидовме да ги евалуираме хипотезите на теоријата за умотза аутизмот, со посебен осврт на дефицитите во когницијата.

References

Astington, J., & Baird, J.(Eds.).(2005).Why language matters for theory of mind. Oxford, UK : Oxford University Press.

Alloway, T.P., Gathercole, S.E., & Pickering, S.J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716.

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).Washington: DC.

Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82.

Asperger, H. (1944). Die ‘aunstisehen Psychopathen’ im Kindesalter. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76–136.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). Perceptual role-taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113–127.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). Are autistic children behaviourists? An examination of their mental-physical and appearance-reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1989c). The autistic child’s theory of mind-a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 30, 285–297.

Baron-Cohen, S., & Goodhart, F. (1994). The “seeing leads to knowing” deficit in autism: The Pratt and Bryant probe. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 397–402.

Baron-Cohen,S., Leslie, A.M.,& Frith, U.(1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?” Cognition, 21, 37-46.

Bettleheim, B. (1967). The empty fortress: Infantile autism and the birth of the self. London: Collier Macmillan.

Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s Child’s talk: Learning to use language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Carlson, S.M.,& Moses, L.J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032-1053.

Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J., & Breton, C. (2002). How specific is the relation between executive function and theory of mind? Contributions ofinhibitory control and working memory. Infant and Child Development, 11, 73–92.

Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J.,& Claxton, L.J. (2004). Individual differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: An investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of Child Experimental Psychology, 87, 299–319.

Diamond, A.,& Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: Development of the abilities to remember what I said and to «Do as I say, not as I do». Developmental Psychobiology, 24, 315–334.

Fine, C., Lumsden, J.,& Blair, R.J.R. (2001). Dissociation between «theory of mind» and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain, 124, 287–298.

Flavell, J.H., Everett, B.A., Croft, K.,& Flavell, E.R. (1981). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1- Level2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17, 99–103.

Flavell, J.H., Flavell, E.R., & Green, F.L. (1983). Development of the appearance – reality distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95–120.

Flavell, J.H., Green, E.R., & Flavell, E.R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. Society for Research in Child Development, 51, 1–87.

Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

Frith,U., & Frith,C.(2003).Development and neuropsychology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London-Series B: Biological Science, 258, 459-473.

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L.,& Rizzolati, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain,119, 593-609.

Happé, F.G.E. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.

Hermelin, B., & O’ Connor, N. (1967). Remembering of words by psychotic and subnormal children. British Journal of Psychology, 58, 213–218.

Hobson, R.P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hove,UK: Erlbaum. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F.(2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London-Series B: Biological Science, 358, 345-360.

Lebedeva, E.I. (2007). Understanding of intentions in situation of deception by children with typical development and autistic children. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 28, 83–89.

McGregor, E., Whiten, A., & Blackburn, P. (1998). Transfer of the picture-inthe- head analogy to natural contexts to aid false belief understanding in autism. Autism, 2, 367–387.

Mundy,P., & Burnette, C.(2005).Joint attention and neurodevelopmental models of autism. In: F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 65–681). New York: Wiley.

Nation, K., & Penny, S. (2008). Sensitivity to eye gaze in autism: Is it normal? Is it automatic? Is it social? Development and Psychopathology, 20, 70– 97.

Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 29, 171–177.

Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B.F., & Rogers, S.J. (1991). Executive function deficit in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.

Ozonoff,S., Cook,I, Coon, H., Dawson, G., Joseph, R. M. Klin, A., et al.(2004). Performance on CANTAB subtests sensitive to frontal lobe function in people with autistic disorder: Evidence from the CPEA Network. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,34,139-150.

Oberman, L., & Ramachandran,V.(2007).The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133,310-327.

Pellicano, E. (2010). Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism. Developmental Psychology, 46, 530–544.

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

Prior, M.R. (1977). Psycholinguistic disabilities of autistic and retarded children. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 21, 37–45.

Prior, M.R., Gajzago, C.C., & Knox, D.T. (1976). Epidemiological-study of autistic and psychotic children in 4 eastern states of Australia. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 10, 173–184.

Peterson,C., Wellman,H., & Liu,D.(2005). Steps in theory of mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76, 502-517.

Ruffman,T., Garnham,W., & Rideout, P.(2001). Social understanding in autism: Eye gaze as a measure of core insight. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42,1083-1094.

Tager-Flusberg,H., & Joseph, R.M.(2005). How language facilitates the acquisition of false belief understanding in children with autism. In J. Astington & J.Baird (Eds.), Why language matters for theory of mind (pp.298-318).Oxford, UK : Oxford University Press.

Serra, M., Loth, F.L., Van Geert, P.L.C., Hurkens, E., & Minderaa, R.B. (2002). Theory of mind in children with ‘lesser variants’ of autism: A longitudinal study.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43, 885–900.

Siegal, M., & Varley, R. (2002). Neural systems involved in «theory of mind». Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 463–471.

Sodian, B., & Frith, U. (1992). Deception and sabotage in autistic, retarded, and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 591–606.

Sodian, B., Taylor, C., Harris, P., & Perner, J. (1992). Early deception and the child’s theory of mind: false trails and genuine markers. Child Development, 62, 468–483.

Wellman, H.M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-ofmind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’ s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.

Wing, L. (1997). The autistic spectrum. Lancet, 350, 1761–1766.

Williams, J.,Waiter, G., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D., Murray, A., & Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and “mirror neuron” functioning in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 44, 610-621.

Zelazo, P.D. (2000). Self-reflection and the development of consciously controlled processing. In: P. Mitchell & K. Riggs (Ed.), Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 169–189). Hove, East Sussex: Psychology Press Ltd.

Published
2013-10-24